Early intervention: What's not to like?


If a child has language problems, when would be the best age
to intervene? At 18 months of age, when they’re just at the outset of learning
language, or at five years, when they’re in school? Most people would say this
is a no-brainer, with early intervention being preferred on two counts:



  • There
    are all kinds of secondary consequences of language difficulties: effects
    on self-esteem, educational outcomes and social interactions. Potentially,
    early intervention can avoid these.

  • It
    is easier to influence the course of development while the brain is still
    plastic. An analogy can be made with vision, where it is well-recognised
    that amblyopia (or "lazy eye") needs to be corrected early in
    life, because otherwise visual pathways in the brain do not develop
    normally, and the potential for good vision in the lazy eye is lost.



Currently, interest by policy-makers in early intervention
has focussed mainly on children’s social and emotional outcomes, with a report by MP Graham Allen emphasising
the benefits, not just for children’s outcomes, but also in economic terms. The
argument is that by preventing problems from developing, we have the potential
to save millions of pounds that would otherwise be spent in dealing with
problems that manifest later in childhood.


The Allen report does not say much about children’s language
development, but similar arguments are often made, and in some areas of the
country, speech and language therapy services put most of their resources into
intervention with preschoolers.


There is, however, a problem with early intervention that is
easily overlooked, but which is well-documented in the case of children’s
language problems. This is the phenomenon of the "late bloomer". Quite simply,
the earlier you identify children’s language difficulties, the higher the
proportion of cases will prove to be "false positives" who spontaneously move
into the normal range without any intervention. We’ve known about this
phenomenon for many years: For instance, a study conducted by Fischel et al in 1989 followed 26
two-year-olds recruited because their parents reported that they understood
complete sentences but could say only a few words.
Five months after initial assessment, one third still had problems, one third
had made some improvement, and one third were in the normal range. Another
study by Thal et al in 1991 followed ten children who scored in the bottom 10% for expressive
vocabulary at the age of 18 to 29 months.
One year after initial assessment, six had caught up, whereas the remaining
four still had delayed language. These early small-scale studies have since
been confirmed by much larger population-based studies in the Netherlands and Australia.


The late-bloomer phenomenon was neatly demonstrated in a study just published in the British Medical Journal by an Australian team
headed by paediatrician Prof Melissa Wake and speech pathologist Prof Sheena Reilly. They
recruited children from a large population-based study, where parents were
asked to complete a Sure Start vocabulary screening measure when their child
was 18 months of age, as well as a child behaviour checklist. Around 20 per
cent of children were reported as having no or very limited spoken words. 301
of these children were randomly allocated to intervention or control groups.
The intervention, "Let's Learn Language", was based on a widely-used approach where parents are trained
to adopt strategies to enhance communicative interactions with their child. The
children were then given a detailed assessment at two years of age, and again
at three years. Results were striking: there were no hints of any difference
between children in the intervention group and control group on any language or
behavioural measures, either at 2 years or at 3 years.


The study authors noted various strengths and weaknesses of
their study. Among these they discussed the possibility that the intensity of
the intervention (six weekly sessions, each lasting 2 hours) may not have been
enough. But they went on to note that “the normal mean language and
vocabulary scores achieved by both intervention and control children by age 3
years suggest that natural resolution, rather than our intervention’s intensity
being too low, explains the null findings.”


They then point out the sobering conclusion to be drawn:
quite simply, if you intervene with children who are likely to improve
spontaneously, there will considerable waste of government’s and families’
resources.


Does this mean we should give up on early intervention? No.
But it does mean that we need to target such intervention much more carefully.
At present, one of the big questions for those of us investigating late talkers
is to find characteristics that will allow us to identify those children who won’t make spontaneous progress. This has
proved to be surprisingly difficult.


Another important message applies to intervention studies
more generally. If you provide an intervention for a condition that
spontaneously improves, it is easy to become convinced that you’ve been
effective. Parents were very positive about the intervention program. There was
remarkably good attendance, and when asked to rate specific features of the
program and its effects, around three quarters of the parents gave positive
responses. This may explain why both parents and professionals find it hard to
believe such interventions have no impact: they do see improvement. Only if you
do a properly controlled trial will the lack of effect become apparent, not
because treated children don’t improve, but rather because the control group
gets better as well.









Reference: (Open Access) :-)

Wake M, Tobin S, Girolametto L, Ukoumunne OC, Gold L, Levickis P, Sheehan J, Goldfeld S, & Reilly S (2011). Outcomes of population based language promotion for slow to talk toddlers at ages 2 and 3 years: Let's Learn Language cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ (Clinical research ed.), 343 PMID: 21852344